The COVID-19 pandemic will undoubtedly be recalled as one of the defining events of the twenty-first century, with preliminary data already demonstrating widespread mental and physical health effects worldwide and throughout Aotearoa New Zealand. Given the gravity and potential longevity of these changes, it is crucial our understanding of wellbeing reflects the changing socio-cultural, financial and political post-pandemic climate.
Full article available to subscribers
The COVID-19 pandemic will undoubtedly be recalled as one of the defining events of the twenty-first century, with preliminary data already demonstrating widespread mental and physical health effects worldwide and throughout Aotearoa New Zealand.1,2 Given the gravity and potential longevity of these changes, it is crucial our understanding of wellbeing reflects the changing socio-cultural, financial and political post-pandemic climate.
The December 2020 quarter wellbeing supplement of the Household Labour Force Survey (Statistics NZ Tatauranga Aotearoa) saw the addition of happiness and anxiety to the biennial survey, a measure of positive and negative affect and relevant emotional correlates of subjective wellbeing.3 This provides an opportunity to better understand how certain factors may relate to anxiety or decreased happiness, particularly during our current social climate. Such knowledge may prove useful in informing future interventions that are more sensitively tailored to the needs and concerns of our current situation.
With the increasing popularity of social determinant models,4 much of Aotearoa’s health research over the past 2 decades has focussed on establishing equity across age, gender, ethnic and socio-economic demographics. However, despite the extensive research this approach has birthed, the relationship between many of these factors and mental wellbeing in Aotearoa has remained poorly understood, with results usually being inconsistent and counterintuitive.
Like almost every developed country, Aotearoa has faced a number of challenges associated with an ageing population.5 These challenges were again brought to the forefront with the primary mortality and morbidity burden of COVID-19 placed disproportionately on older populations.6 It has also spurred a growing discourse that considers the ways ageing populations are discussed and portrayed, especially in the media.7 Previous research reporting the counterintuitive finding that wellbeing improves with age has garnered such attention that the phenomenon has become well-known in popular culture, dubbed “the paradox of ageing”.8 While the findings have been replicated, so too have they been critiqued and contradicted, with many outlining that such a pattern should not be considered paradoxical, that no such pattern exists, or indeed that older populations may be less happy and more anxious.8–12 However, any association is likely a complex one, and thus it is unknown how a pandemic environment has influenced this, or will continue to shape wellbeing in older age. Given the potential long-term physiological and sociological effects that the pandemic may bring, investigating its effects on ageing populations will likely be of increasing importance.1,13,14
In addition to the direct effects of social isolation, many of the chronic or acute illnesses associated with ageing became more difficult to manage with increased barriers to healthcare and medication throughout lockdowns.15,16 Ageing is a multifactorial and complex process, influenced by a number of convoluted cultural, psychosocial and physiological interactions.17 Thus, the full effects of the pandemic on older populations have not yet been fully appreciated. This provides a strong rationale to track the wellbeing of older adult populations throughout, and perhaps beyond, the COVID-19 pandemic.
Physical health status and mental health status are undoubtedly closely intertwined, with numerous bidirectional associations having been previously outlined.18 Given the links between loneliness, social isolation and the constant threat of nationwide lockdown, the contribution of isolation to happiness and anxiety will likely be particularly prominent in the coming years, especially for communities with lower physical health.19 Similarly, disabled communities typically record higher rates of social isolation, alongside an increased number of mental health challenges.20 As with elderly populations, stress associated with the management of chronic or acute illness may be amplified by isolations and difficulties accessing healthcare.
Gender difference is perhaps one of the most thoroughly explored topics in mental health fields, with literature—although often conflicting—generally supporting the idea that women experience higher rates of negative affect, such as anxiety or depressive symptoms, and yet are typically happier than men.21–23 While we are yet to fully understand the impact of COVID-19 on such gender and mental health interactions, preliminary studies have suggested that participants reporting as female have experienced higher rates of distress during the COVID-19 pandemic than male-reporting participants.24 In addition to the existing disparity in anxiety for women, a number of gender-related stressors have been influenced by the pandemic environment, such as increased violence against women and higher mental health risk for peripartum women.25–27 Some studies have, however, found no difference between genders, and a Netherlands cohort showed women experiencing higher rates of anxiety, but men having higher rates of depression.28–30 Thus, while gender continues to be a relevant factor in discussions of mental wellbeing, such associations have remained unclear, and we will likely continue to uncover new modifying factors within the post-COVID climate.
A number of socio-economic factors are strongly intertwined with mental wellbeing, with income, housing and employment each contributing significantly to subjective wellbeing.31–33 Given the nationwide economic and commercial effects of COVID-19, struggles with job security, housing and employment may have contributed even more strongly to anxiety and happiness scores.
Previous studies have also disagreed about the nature of ethnic effects on mental wellbeing, with Māori and Pākehā populations having been contrastingly described as the group with the highest rate of mood disorders.34,35 Further analysis of what factors may be contributing to feelings of anxiety and happiness may be central in understanding these apparent disagreements within the data, and better appreciating the unique challenges experienced by the different facets of Aotearoa’s increasingly multicultural population. And while we are not yet able to fully appreciate the broad scope of differences between ethnic group responses to the COVID-19 pandemic, given that socio-economic structure, workforce distribution, likelihood of hospitalisation and perspective on disease and vaccination all differ by ethnicity,36,37 it is likely that different responses to the pandemic will become more apparent as we move forward.
Finally, trust on both institutional and personal levels have been previously shown to influence feelings of anxiety.38–39 This has clear relevance to the pandemic’s effects, which were, of course, not solely biomedical, having effected dramatic socio-cultural and political change on a global scale.1 Trust in the government and in the general populace has become actively discussed and critiqued on a daily basis, which may have influenced how strongly trust contributed to feelings of anxiety and happiness within this dataset. All these factors were considered for their potential association with anxiety and happiness scores in Aotearoa in December 2020.
We conducted a cross-sectional study to identify the possible contributors to the mental health status of New Zealanders in December 2020. We used the data sample collected in the wellbeing survey, where up to 30,000 responders were contacted from across Aotearoa as part of the Household Labour Force Survey (HLFS).
We conducted two separate analyses on the mental wellbeing outcome variables of happiness and anxiety. The use of two independent models allows us to assess contributing factors to both a positive aspect of mental wellbeing (i.e., happiness, from the answer to the question “How happy were you yesterday?” on a scale of 0–10) and a negative aspect of mental wellbeing (i.e., anxiety, from the answer to the question “How anxious were you yesterday?” on a scale of 0–10) as unvalidated measures of happiness and anxiety. Notably, the study data were collected in the midst of the global COVID-19 pandemic, and thus the results of this observational study taken at a single time point reflect the factors that were related to mental health within Aotearoa amid this pandemic, but at a time where most New Zealanders were able to undergo relatively normal activities under Alert Level 1 (international borders were closed).
The predictor variables for each of the models constituted the key wellbeing measures provided within the HLFS, including:
We firstly quantified a full correlation matrix between all variables within the statistical programming software R to assess the associations between the identified factors. Multiple linear regression models were then fit for both anxiety and happiness scores, with each of the predictor variables listed above. Variables were Z-scored prior to entry into the model, such that the magnitude of each of the covariates could be reported and compared.
The significance of each of the predictor variables were assessed using a threshold of p <0.05, with additional Bonferroni correction to account for the number of predictors included within the model. Both corrected and uncorrected values are reported, with significance values that survive only the uncorrected threshold labelled as exploratory results.
The respondents to the wellbeing supplement of the HLFS in December 2020 totalled 12,465. All counts below 6 have been suppressed, and randomly rounded to a base of three according to the output rules of Statistics New Zealand, to maintain data confidentiality. A summary of the data is provided in Tables 1 and 2.
View Table 1–4, Figure 1.
Greater anxiety scores were found to be most strongly related to smaller happiness scores, greater loneliness, poorer physical health, poorer family wellbeing and poorer financial wellbeing. Alongside a negative correlation between happiness and anxiety, greater happiness scores were found to be most strongly related to greater family wellbeing, greater physical health, smaller loneliness scores, greater financial wellbeing, and greater generalised trust, trust in the police and trust in the health system. A depiction of the full correlation matrix results is presented in Figure 1, and R and p-values can be found in the Appendices.
The variables most strongly associated with self-reported anxiety using multiple linear regression were age, sex (female), discrimination, poor physical health, loneliness, unemployment status, poorer financial wellbeing and poorer family wellbeing. Other significant but more weakly associated variables include disability, damp housing, playing a parental role, less trust in the police and more trust in the media. While participants who identified as Māori or Other were associated with a p-value <0.05, these did not survive Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons. The complete list of regression parameters is provided in Table 3.
The variables most strongly associated with self-reported happiness using multiple linear regression were age, ethnicity (Māori and Pacific peoples), financial wellbeing and family wellbeing. Greater levels of generalised trust, trust in the police and trust in the health system also correlated with greater happiness scores. Contrastingly, poor physical health, being a parent and both being employed or unemployed (as opposed to not in the labour force, such as retired populations) were related to lower reports of happiness. Weaker associations were found between happiness, recording sex as female, lower reported discrimination and being without a disability. A full list of results is provided in Table 4.
Here we investigated the relationships between anxiety and happiness with a host of social, physical, financial and mental health variables in Aotearoa, in the midst of the COVID-19 pandemic (December 2020). As expected, happiness and anxiety were inversely related, and shared a number of contributing variables. Loneliness, physical health, family wellbeing, financial wellbeing, age and gender all had associations with both anxiety and happiness. Furthermore, these results provide a platform for future longitudinal studies that may gauge social progressions in equity and wellbeing for Aotearoa.
Aligning with previous literature, older adults reported higher levels of happiness and lower levels of anxiety.40,41 One leading hypothesis suggests that mental wellbeing in older adults may be more intrinsically generated rather than dependent on specific extrinsic stimuli, with greater emotional control possibly adaptive in combating the physical and social challenges of ageing.42 Such a perspective may explain why, despite the added and disproportionate stress COVID-19 placed on older populations, better mental wellbeing was still observed. This was supported by the regression analysis, with higher happiness and lower anxiety scores associated with age that persisted beyond the exclusion of employment status, financial wellbeing and a number of other extrinsic contributors to mental wellbeing. Additionally, the results also accounted for many of the potential insults to mental wellbeing in older adults, including loneliness, physical health and discrimination. Useful future research might include monitoring changes within populations reaching older age in a post-pandemic environment.
Our gender analyses also supported previous literature, with women reporting higher rates of anxiety,43 with gender being one of the main associated factors in the anxiety regression. However, being female was also weakly associated with greater happiness in the regression analysis. It is unclear how this was influenced by the pandemic, however, due to the pandemic’s known effects on gender-related inequities such as violence, perinatal mental illness and employment.25–27 It should also be noted that this study was restricted to a binary view of gender and therefore may not appreciate the full scope of mental wellbeing within non-binary populations.
One of the most prominent factors associated with both anxiety and happiness was physical health. This coincides with previous findings demonstrating links between mental and physical wellbeing, with many concluding that this relationship is likely complex and bidirectional.44 It is also possible that the links between physical and mental health may have been intensified by COVID-19’s particular threat to populations with poor physical health, potentially exacerbating this relationship further.
Loneliness, both during and before the pandemic, has been clearly associated with higher anxiety and lower happiness.45,46 This was certainly supported by our results, with loneliness being the factor with the highest association to anxiety, and among the highest (inverse) correlates of happiness. Perhaps counterintuitively, during the pandemic, younger populations were more likely to report high levels of loneliness.47 Our data showed similar patterns, with age being inversely associated with loneliness. However, older populations have also seen an increase in loneliness during the pandemic.48 The impacts of loneliness predictably affect selective demographics, being correlated with lower financial wellbeing, poorer physical health and disability. Being Pākehā or male was also protective against loneliness; however, these correlations were more subtle. Like with many other factors, while the effects may not be immediately evident, large aspects of social routine have been, and continue to be, disrupted. Evidence has shown such disruptions may have widespread mental and physical health effects, again underscoring the cruciality of monitoring our adjustment to post-pandemic society.49
The impacts of financial wellbeing and socio-economic status have been among the most studied areas in the last few decades, spanning a number of disciplines.50,51 Unsurprisingly, the far-reaching effects of financial wellbeing have been found to contribute to mental wellbeing during pandemic times in a number of countries.52–54 However, due to the stark differences in healthcare, economic and social welfare systems, it is unknown whether such results are valid when applied internationally and cross-culturally. One study, in fact, observed greater decline in happiness among higher socio-economic groups.55 Our results showed financial wellbeing as significantly correlated with happiness and inversely correlated with anxiety. While associations of damp or cold housing status did not survive Bonferroni correction, given the overlap between financial wellbeing and housing quality, much of the socio-economically relevant variance was likely accounted for by the overall financial wellbeing correlate. While employment had a small association with anxiety, unemployment demonstrated a stronger correlation. Contrastingly, employment was associated more strongly with unhappiness during the pandemic than was unemployment, while pre-pandemic studies describe employment as positively associated with happiness.56 However, with the turbulent work environment brought on by the pandemic, many were required to work from home and increased burden was often placed on those whose work was deemed essential in Aotearoa. It should be noted, however, that studies from Israel and South Africa have outlined employment as protective against distress and poor mental health.57,58 This may highlight the need for interventional measures to counteract challenges to mental wellbeing in post-pandemic professional environments.
Family wellbeing was strongly associated with both decreased anxiety and increased happiness, where nationwide household isolations may have further strengthened the impact of family on wellbeing. This association is not specific to pandemic times, with a number of studies outlining a clear association between family wellbeing and overall wellbeing, with many reporting family wellbeing as more important than personal wellbeing.59 Poor family wellbeing was also associated with a number of other relevant factors including poor physical health, loneliness and decreased trust in all measured areas.
While no ethnic population findings survived Bonferroni correction in the anxiety regression, it is worth noting that discrimination was among the strongest of anxiety correlates and was correlated with identifying as Māori or Pacific peoples. The other primary correlates of anxiety were poor physical health, poor financial wellbeing, poor family wellbeing and loneliness, each of which was higher in Māori populations. Similarly, higher than average proportions of Pacific populations reported poor physical health and poor financial wellbeing. In contrast, the regression analyses demonstrated that being Māori or Pacific peoples was, in fact, associated with higher happiness scores. As with anxiety, this is after accounting for many ethnically distributed happiness correlates such as the influence of physical health, loneliness and family wellbeing. Interestingly, discrimination, while associated with anxiety, was not significant in its association with happiness after Bonferroni correction. These results highlight that the racial inequalities in mental wellbeing that exist in Aotearoa are likely deeply preventable, and potentially stem from a number of inadequacies within our social, economic and cultural structures.
Furthermore, reports have shown that while Māori, Pākehā and Pacific populations reported similar rates of psychological distress throughout lockdown, recovery after lockdown was decreased in Māori and Pacific communities.60 Asian communities, however, reported less psychological distress during lockdown.61 It is unknown how this may affect contributing factors in happiness and anxiety between ethnic groups; however, if such trends continue, we may see ethnicity as more strongly associated with anxiety and happiness in the coming years, as lagging effects on wellbeing and recovery time become more apparent.
Finally, no measured trust variables showed significant independent associations with anxiety in the regression model. However, higher rates of generalised trust, trust in the police and trust in the health system were all significantly associated with higher levels of happiness. This aligns with past research on trust in Japanese populations, which has demonstrated an association between individual and social trust, and happiness.38,39 However, trust in parliament and media showed no significant independent association in the current analyses, despite the constant, daily communication from parliament. While media transparency has perhaps never been a more salient issue in this country’s history, it is possible that this effect was accounted for by other significant trust measures.
The COVID-19 pandemic posed a number of novel socio-cultural, psychological and political challenges on a global scale. While much research has been conducted both during and after the pandemic to understand the ramifications of this, there has been limited documentation of how different demographics, attitudes, gender and ethnicity intersected with mental wellbeing in Aotearoa at a time of national crisis. While these cross-sectional analyses can only measure association rather than causation, it is hoped this will provide a valuable snapshot of national wellbeing. Moreover, it will prove valuable to compare this dataset with future trends in data, to aid our understanding of how the social and demographic determinants of mental wellbeing may differ during times of crises, such that future measures can be effectively and sensitively tailored to the needs of different subpopulations within Aotearoa.
View Appendices.
Mental wellbeing has been one of the most prominent health concerns in Aotearoa New Zealand and has only been exacerbated by the COVID-19 pandemic. Here we explored factors associated with anxiety and happiness in a mid-pandemic climate in Aotearoa.
Analyses were performed on the anxiety and happiness scores from the wellbeing survey in December 2020 (Statistics NZ Tatauranga Aotearoa; 30,000 responders contacted for the Household Labour Force Survey). Correlations and general linear models were used to identify significant predictor variables related to anxiety and happiness scores.
A number of factors correlated with both anxiety and happiness, including loneliness, physical health, family wellbeing, financial wellbeing, age and gender. After controlling for many ethnically stratified social burdens, Māori and Pacific populations demonstrated higher levels of happiness. Discrimination was only associated with anxiety, while generalised trust, trust in the police and in the health system all related to happiness.
Anxiety and happiness in a mid-pandemic environment shared many related variables spanning physical, social and financial domains. Additionally, anxiety was associated with greater levels of discrimination, and happiness with trust in public services. Here we provide a window into the state of mental wellbeing in Aotearoa during a global health crisis.
Mana Mitchell: Kōhatu Centre for Hauora Māori, University of Otago, New Zealand; Otago Medical School, University of Otago, New Zealand; Ngāti Maniapoto, New Zealand.
Bradley I Tomlinson: School of Pharmacy, University of Otago, New Zealand.
Grace O Egan: School of Pharmacy, University of Otago, New Zealand.
Yanming Huang: School of Pharmacy, University of Otago, New Zealand.
Emily C Kellett: School of Pharmacy, University of Otago, New Zealand.
George P F Rowley: School of Pharmacy, University of Otago, New Zealand.
Sophie I Tang: School of Pharmacy, University of Otago, New Zealand.
Ysobel G Maindonald: School of Pharmacy, University of Otago, New Zealand; Tapuika, New Zealand; Waitaha, New Zealand.
Ellie M Logan: School of Pharmacy, University of Otago, New Zealand; Waikato-Tainui, New Zealand.
Bruce R Russell: School of Pharmacy, University of Otago, New Zealand.
Nicholas Bowden: Department of Women’s and Children’s Health, University of Otago, New Zealand.
Olivia K Harrison: Department of Psychology, University of Otago, New Zealand.
Mana Mitchell (Ngāti Maniapoto): Kōhatu and Otago Medical School, University of Otago.
Olivia K Harrison (née Faull): Department of Psychology, University of Otago.
Nil.
Rutherford Discovery Fellowship from the Royal Society of New Zealand (awarded to Dr Harrison).
School of Pharmacy, University of Otago - Provision of STATSNZ access funding.
1) Matthewman S, Huppatz K. A sociology of Covid-19. J Sociol. 2020;56(4):675-83. doi: 10.1177/1440783320939416.
2) Cumming J. Going hard and early: Aotearoa New Zealand’s response to Covid-19. Health Econ Policy Law. 2022;17(1):107-119. doi: 10.1017/S174413312100013X.
3) Das KV, Jones-Harrell C, Fan Y, et al. Understanding subjective well-being: perspectives from psychology and public health. Public Health Rev. 2020;41(1):25. doi: 10.1186/s40985-020-00142-5.
4) Lundberg O. Next steps in the development of the social determinants of health approach: the need for a new narrative. Scand J Public Health. 2020;48(5):473-9. doi: 10.1177/1403494819894789.
5) Parr-Brownlie LC, Waters DL, Neville S, et al. Aging in New Zealand: Ka haere ki te ao pakeketanga. Gerontologist. 2020;60(5):812-20. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnaa032. Erratum in: Gerontologist. 2021;61(5):805. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnaa110.
6) Jefferies S, French N, Gilkison C, et al. COVID-19 in New Zealand and the impact of the national response: a descriptive epidemiological study. Lancet Public Health. 2020;5(11):e612-23. doi: 10.1016/S2468-2667(20)30225-5.
7) Morgan T, Wiles J, Williams L, Gott M. COVID-19 and the portrayal of older people in New Zealand news media. J R Soc N Z. 2021;51(S1):S127-42. doi: 10.1080/03036758.2021.1884098.
8) Schilling O. Development of Life Satisfaction in Old Age: Another View on the “Paradox”. Soc Indic Res. 2006;75(2):241-71. doi: 10.1007/s11205-004-5297-2.
9) Hansen T, Slagsvold B. The age and subjective well-being paradox revisited: A multidimensional perspective. Norsk Epidemiologi. 2012;22(2):187-95. doi: 10.5324/nje.v22i2.1565.
10) Hellevik O. Ottar Hellevik: Jakten på den norske lykken. Norsk Monitor 1985-2007. Sosiologi i dag. 2009;39(3):94-107.
11) Eriksen J, Naess S, Moum T. Livskvalitet: Forskning om det gode liv. NO: Fagbokforlaget; 2011.
12) Tseng HY, Löckenhoff C, Lee CY, et al. The paradox of aging and health-related quality of life in Asian Chinese: results from the Healthy Aging Longitudinal Study in Taiwan. BMC Geriatr. 2020;20(1):91. doi: 10.1186/s12877-020-1446-y.
13) The Lancet. Understanding long COVID: a modern medical challenge. Lancet. 2021;398(10302):725. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(21)01900-0.
14) Taquet M, Dercon Q, Luciano S, et al. Incidence, co-occurrence, and evolution of long-COVID features: A 6-month retrospective cohort study of 273,618 survivors of COVID-19. PLoS Med. 2021;18(9):e1003773. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1003773.
15) Radwan E, Radwan A, Radwan W. Challenges Facing Older Adults during the COVID-19 Outbreak. Eur J Environ Public Health. 2020;5(1):em0059. doi: 10.29333/ejeph/8457.
16) Sepúlveda-Loyola W, Rodríguez-Sánchez I, Pérez-Rodríguez P, et al. Impact of Social Isolation Due to COVID-19 on Health in Older People: Mental and Physical Effects and Recommendations. J Nutr Health Aging. 2020;24(9):938-47. doi: 10.1007/s12603-020-1469-2.
17) Michel JP, Dreux C, Vacheron A. Healthy ageing: Evidence that improvement is possible at every age. Eur Geriatr Med. 2016;7(4):298-305. doi: 10.1016/j.eurger.2016.04.014.
18) Ohrnberger J, Fichera E, Sutton M. The relationship between physical and mental health: A mediation analysis. Soc Sci Med. 2017;195:42-9. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.11.008.
19) Khosravi M. COVID-19 quarantine: Two-way interaction between physical activity and mental health. Eur J Transl Myol. 2021;30(4):9509. doi: 10.4081/ejtm.2020.9509.
20) Heinze N, Hussain SF, Castle CL, et al. The Long-Term Impact of the COVID-19 Pandemic on Loneliness in People Living With Disability and Visual Impairment. Front Public Health. 2021;9:1-11. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2021.738304.
21) Batz-Barbarich C, Tay L. Gender Differences in Subjective Well-Being. Handbook of well-being. 2017;1-15.
22) Eaton NR, Keyes KM, Krueger RF, et al. An Invariant Dimensional Liability Model of Gender Differences in Mental Disorder Prevalence: Evidence from a National Sample. J Abnorm Psychol. 2012;121(1):282-8. doi: 10.1037/a0024780.
23) Zweig J. Are Women Happier than Men? Evidence from the Gallup World Poll. J Happiness Stud. 2014;16(2):515-541.
24) Heffner J, Vives ML, FeldmanHall O. Anxiety, gender, and social media consumption predict COVID-19 emotional distress. Humanit Soc Sci Commun. 2021;8(1). doi: 10.1057/s41599-021-00816-8.
25) Fisseha S, Sen G, Ghebreyesus TA, et al. COVID-19: the turning point for gender equality. The Lancet. 2021;398(10299):471-4.
26) Almeida M, Shrestha AD, Stojanac D, Miller LJ. The impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on women’s mental health. Arch Womens Ment Health. 2020;23(6):741-8. doi: 10.1007/s00737-020-01092-2.
27) Bourgault S, Peterman A, O’Donnell M. Violence Against Women and Children During COVID-19—One Year On and 100 Papers In, A Fourth Research Round Up [Internet]. Center for Global Development; 2021 [cited 2024 Apr 30]. Available from: https://www.cgdev.org/sites/default/files/vawc-fourth-roundup.pdf
28) Rania N, Coppola I. Psychological Impact of the Lockdown in Italy Due to the COVID-19 Outbreak: Are There Gender Differences? Front Psychol. 2021;12:567470. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.567470.
29) Stieger S, Lewetz D, Swami V. Emotional Well-Being Under Conditions of Lockdown: An Experience Sampling Study in Austria During the COVID-19 Pandemic. J Happiness Stud. 2021;22(6):2703-20. doi: 10.1007/s10902-020-00337-2.
30) Vloo A, Alessie RJM, Mierau JO, et al. Gender differences in the mental health impact of the COVID-19 lockdown: Longitudinal evidence from the Netherlands. SSM Popul Health. 2021;15:100878. doi: 10.1016/j.ssmph.2021.100878.
31) Kahneman D, Deaton A. High income improves evaluation of life but not emotional well-being. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107(38):16489-93. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1011492107.
32) Bond L, Kearns A, Mason P, et al. Exploring the relationships between housing, neighbourhoods and mental wellbeing for residents of deprived areas. BMC Public Health. 2012;12:48. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-12-48.
33) Jebb AT, Morrison M, Tay L, Diener E. Subjective Well-Being Around the World: Trends and Predictors Across the Life Span. Psychol Sci. 2020;31(3):293-305. doi: 10.1177/0956797619898826.
34) Baxter J, Kokaua J, Wells JE, et al. Ethnic Comparisons of the 12 Month Prevalence of Mental Disorders and Treatment Contact in Te Rau Hinengaro: the New Zealand Mental Health Survey. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2006;40(10):905-13. doi: 10.1080/j.1440-1614.2006.01910.x.
35) Chow CS, Mulder RT. Mental health service use by Asians: a New Zealand census. N Z Med J. 2017;130(1461):35-41.
36) Lee CHJ, Sibley CG. Attitudes toward vaccinations are becoming more polarized in New Zealand: Findings from a longitudinal survey. EClinicalMedicine. 2020;23:100387.
37) Steyn N, Binny RN, Hannah K, et al. Māori and Pacific people in New Zealand have a higher risk of hospitalisation for COVID-19. N Z Med J. 2021;134(1538):28-43.
38) Tokuda Y, Fujii S, Inoguchi T. Individual and Country‐Level Effects of Social Trust on Happiness: The Asia Barometer Survey. J Appl Soc Psychol. 2010;40:2574-93.
39) Kuroki M. Does social trust increase individual happiness in Japan? Jpn Econ Rev. 2011;62(4):444-59.
40) Chaudhary S, Zhang S, Zhornitsky S, et al. Age-related reduction in trait anxiety: Behavioral and neural evidence of automaticity in negative facial emotion processing. Neuroimage. 2023;276:120207. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2023.120207.
41) Hansen T, Blekesaune M. The age and well-being “paradox”: a longitudinal and multidimensional reconsideration. Eur J Ageing. 2022;19(4):1277-86.
42) Carstensen LL, Turan B, Scheibe S, et al. Emotional Experience Improves With Age: Evidence Based on Over 10 Years of Experience Sampling. Psychol Aging. 2011;26(1):21-33. doi: 10.1037/a0021285.
43) McLean CP, Asnaani A, Litz BT, Hofmann SG. Gender differences in anxiety disorders: Prevalence, course of illness, comorbidity and burden of illness. J Psychiatr Res. 2011;45(8):1027-35. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2011.03.006.
44) Steptoe A. Happiness and Health. Annu Rev Public Health. 2019;40:339-59. doi: 10.1146/annurev-publhealth-040218-044150.
45) Mushtaq R, Shoib S, Shah T, Mushtaq S. Relationship between loneliness, Psychiatric disorders and physical health ? A review on the psychological aspects of loneliness. J Clin Diagn Res. 2014;8(9):WE01-4. doi: 10.7860/JCDR/2014/10077.4828.
46) Beutel ME, Klein EM, Brähler E, et al. Loneliness in the general population: Prevalence, determinants and relations to mental health. BMC Psychiatry. 2017;17(1):97. doi: 10.1186/s12888-017-1262-x.
47) Wickens CM, McDonald AJ, Elton-Marshall T, et al. Loneliness in the COVID-19 pandemic: Associations with age, gender and their interaction. J Psychiatr Res. 2021;136:103-8. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2021.01.047.
48) Heidinger T, Richter L. The Effect of COVID-19 on Loneliness in the Elderly. An Empirical Comparison of Pre-and Peri-Pandemic Loneliness in Community-Dwelling Elderly. Front Psychol. 2020;11:585308. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.585308.
49) Cai D, Zhu M, Lin M, et al. The bidirectional relationship between positive mental health and social rhythm in college students: A three-year longitudinal study. Front Psychol. 2017;8:1119. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01119.
50) Farah MJ. The Neuroscience of Socioeconomic Status: Correlates, Causes, and Consequences. Neuron. 2017;96(1):56-71. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.08.034.
51) Navarro-Carrillo G, Alonso-Ferres M, Moya M, Valor-Segura I. Socioeconomic Status and Psychological Well-Being: Revisiting the Role of Subjective Socioeconomic Status. Front Psychol. 2020;11:1303. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01303.
52) OECD. Tackling the mental health impact of the COVID-19 crisis: An integrated, whole-of-society response [Internet]. Paris (FR): OECD Publishing; 2021 [cited 2024 Apr 30]. Available from: https://www.oecd-ilibrary.org/social-issues-migration-health/tackling-the-mental-health-impact-of-the-covid-19-crisis-an-integrated-whole-of-society-response_0ccafa0b-en
53) Nagasu M, Muto K, Yamamoto I. Impacts of anxiety and socioeconomic factors on mental health in the early phases of the COVID-19 pandemic in the general population in Japan: A web-based survey. PLoS One. 2021;16(3):e0247705. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0247705.
54) Gazmararian J, Weingart R, Campbell K, et al. Impact of COVID-19 Pandemic on the Mental Health of Students From 2 Semi-Rural High Schools in Georgia*. J Sch Health. 2021;91(5):356-69. doi: 10.1111/josh.13007.
55) Wanberg CR, Csillag B, Douglass RP, et al. Socioeconomic status and well-being during COVID-19: A resource-based examination. J Appl Psychol. 2020;105(12):1382-96. doi: 10.1037/apl0000831.
56) Winkelmann R. Unemployment and happiness. IZA World of Labor. 2014;94:1-10. doi: 10.15185/izawol.94.
57) Achdut N, Refaeli T. Unemployment and psychological distress among young people during the COVID‐19 pandemic: Psychological resources and risk factors. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2020;17(19):7163. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17197163.
58) Posel D, Oyenubi A, Kollamparambil U. Job loss and mental health during the COVID-19 lockdown: Evidence from South Africa. PLoS One. 2021;16(3):e0249352. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0249352.
59) Krys K, Capaldi CA, Zelenski JM, et al. Family well-being is valued more than personal well-being: A four-country study. Curr Psychol. 2021;40(1-23):3332-43. doi: 10.1007/s12144-019-00249-2.
60) Nicolson MN, Flett JA. The mental wellbeing of New Zealanders during and post-lockdown. N Z Med J. 2020;133(1523):110-12.
61) Every-Palmer S, Jenkins M, Gendall P, et al. Psychological distress, anxiety, family violence, suicidality, and wellbeing in New Zealand during the COVID-19 lockdown: A cross-sectional study. PLoS One. 2020;15(11):e0241658. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0241658.
Sign in to view your account and access
the latest publications by the NZMJ.
Don't have an account?
Let's get started with creating an account.
Already have an account?
Become a member to enjoy unlimited digital access and support the ongoing publication of the New Zealand Medical Journal.
The New Zealand Medical Journal is fully available to individual subscribers and does not incur a subscription fee. This applies to both New Zealand and international subscribers. Institutions are encouraged to subscribe. The value of institutional subscriptions is essential to the NZMJ, as supporting a reputable medical journal demonstrates an institution’s commitment to academic excellence and professional development. By continuing to pay for a subscription, institutions signal their support for valuable medical research and contribute to the journal's continued success.
Please email us at nzmj@pmagroup.co.nz